Thursday, July 16, 2020

The Role of Classical Conditioning in Taste Aversions

The Role of Classical Conditioning in Taste Aversions Theories Behavioral Psychology Print Avoidance of Certain Foods and Classical Conditioning By Kendra Cherry facebook twitter Kendra Cherry, MS, is an author, educational consultant, and speaker focused on helping students learn about psychology. Learn about our editorial policy Kendra Cherry Medically reviewed by Medically reviewed by Steven Gans, MD on January 14, 2016 Steven Gans, MD is board-certified in psychiatry and is an active supervisor, teacher, and mentor at Massachusetts General Hospital. Learn about our Medical Review Board Steven Gans, MD Updated on August 16, 2019 Jose Luis Pelaez Inc / Blend Images / Getty Images More in Theories Behavioral Psychology Cognitive Psychology Developmental Psychology Personality Psychology Social Psychology Biological Psychology Psychosocial Psychology A conditioned taste aversion involves the avoidance of a certain food following a period of illness after consuming that food. These aversions are a great example of how classical conditioning can result in changes in behavior, even after only one incidence of feeling ill. How Do Conditioned Taste Aversions Work? Have you ever gotten ill after eating something and later found that just the thought of that food made you feel a bit queasy? This is a great example of what is often referred to as a conditioned taste aversion. A conditioned taste aversion can occur when eating a substance is followed by illness. For example, if you ate sushi for lunch and then became ill, you might avoid eating sushi in the future, even if it had no relationship to your illness. While it might seem expected that we would avoid foods that were immediately followed by illness, research has shown that the consumption of the food and the onset of the illness do not need to necessarily occur close together. Conditioned taste aversions can develop even when there is a long delay between the neutral stimulus (eating the food) and the unconditioned stimulus (feeling sick). In classical conditioning, conditioned food aversions are examples of single-trial learning. Just one pairing of the previously neutral stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus can establish an automatic response. Examples of Conditioned Taste Aversion Imagine that you are on vacation and eat a chicken enchilada at a restaurant. Hours after eating the enchilada, you become violently ill. For years after that incident, you might be unable to bring yourself to eat a chicken enchilada and may even feet queasy when you smell foods that remind you of that particular dish. This conditioned taste aversion can occur even when you know that your illness is not connected to eating that particular item. In reality, you might be fully aware that you picked up a nasty stomach virus from one of your traveling companions who had been ill just days before the trip. Consider your own aversions to certain foods. Can you link your distaste for particular items to a period of illness, queasiness, or nausea? People may find that they avoid very specific types of food for years simply because they consumed that particular item before they  became ill. Conditioned taste aversions are quite common and can last for days to several years. Understanding Taste Aversions Can taste aversions occur both consciously and unconsciously? In many cases, people may be completely unaware of the underlying reasons for their dislike of a type of food. Why do these taste aversions occur, especially when we consciously realize that the illness was not tied to a particular food? Conditioned taste aversions are a great example of some of the fundamental mechanics of classical conditioning. The previously neutral stimulus (the food) is paired with an unconditioned stimulus (an illness), which leads to an unconditioned response (feeling sick).After this one-time pairing, the previously neutral stimulus (the food) is now a conditioned stimulus that elicits a conditioned response (avoiding the food). Is that all there is to these conditioned taste aversions? The scenario described above does not exactly fit with the standard expectations for classical conditioning. First of all, the conditioning occurred after just a single pairing of the neutral stimulus and unconditioned stimulus (UCS). Second, the time span between the neutral stimulus and UCS is usually just a matter of seconds. In the case of a conditioned taste aversion, the time-lapse often amounts to several hours. While it may seem to violate the general principles of classical conditioning, researchers have been able to demonstrate the effects of conditioned taste aversions in experimental settings. In one such experiment, psychologist John Garcia fed flavored water (a previously neutral stimulus) to lab rats. Several hours later, the rats were injected with a substance (the UCS) that made them ill. Later, when the rats were offered flavored water, they refused to drink it. Explaining Conditioned Taste Aversions Because GarciaĆ¢€™s research contradicted much of what was previously understood about classical conditioning, many psychologists were unconvinced by the results. Pavlov had suggested that any neutral stimulus could elicit a conditioned response. If that were true, then why would the feelings of sickness be associated with the food that was eaten hours earlier? Wouldnt the illness be associated with something that had happened right before the symptoms occurred? Taste aversions do not fit comfortably within the present framework of classical or instrumental conditioning, Garcia noted. These aversions selectively seek flavors to the exclusion of other stimuli. Interstimulus intervals are a thousand-fold too long. What Garcia and other researchers were able to demonstrate was that in some cases, the type of neutral stimulus used does have an influence on the conditioning process. So why does the type of stimulus matter so much in this particular case? One part of the explanation lies in the concept of biological preparedness. Essentially, virtually every organism is biologically predisposed to create certain associations between certain stimuli. If an animal eats food and then becomes ill, it might be very important to the animals continued existence to avoid such foods in the future. These associations are frequently essential for survival, so it is no wonder they form easily. A Word From Verywell Classical conditioning can have a powerful influence on behavior. As conditioned taste aversions so clearly demonstrate, sometimes learning can occur very quickly (after only a single instance). The next time you find yourself avoiding a particular food, consider the role that a conditioned aversion may have played in your dislike for that particular item.